Show simple item record

dc.contributor.authorBostrom, Ann
dc.contributor.authorBöhm, Gisela
dc.contributor.authorO'Connor, Robert
dc.contributor.authorHanss, Daniel
dc.contributor.authorBodi-Fernandez, Otto
dc.contributor.authorHalder, Pradipta
dc.date.accessioned2023-08-03T08:44:41Z
dc.date.available2023-08-03T08:44:41Z
dc.date.created2021-01-24T19:05:56Z
dc.date.issued2020
dc.identifier.citationJournal of Risk Research. 2020, 23 (7-8), 902-911.en_US
dc.identifier.issn1366-9877
dc.identifier.urihttps://hdl.handle.net/11250/3082453
dc.description© 2020 Informa UK Limited, trading as Taylor & Francis Group. Dette er den aksepterte versjonen av en artikkel publisert i Journal of Risk Research. Du finner den publiserte artikkelen her: https://doi.org/10.1080/13669877.2020.1756384 / This is the postprint version of the article published in Journal of Risk Research. You can find the published article here. https://doi.org/10.1080/13669877.2020.1756384en_US
dc.description.abstractJudgment, decision making, and risk researchers have learned a great deal over the years about how people prepare for and react to global risks. In recent years, risk scholars have increasingly focused their energies on climate change, and as pandemic coronavirus has swept the globe many of these scholars are comparing the coronavirus pandemic with climate change to inform risk management. Risk communication research and the best practices developed from it are predicated on findings from the 1970’s to the present showing that there are structural similarities in how people think about widely divergent risks. Consequently, these lessons from risk communication of climate change (and from the canon of best practices) apply to the coronavirus pandemic. In the empirical comparison of student perceptions reported here, we replicate these structural similarity findings, but also find that moral concerns in particular deserve attention as a potentially distinct dimension of risk perception, on which different risks may also differ, as pandemic risks appear to evoke less moral concern than climate change. The need for communications to be timely, honest, credible, empathetic, and informative for useful individual actions is fundamental and essential for communicating effectively about the coronavirus epidemic. Some countries have heeded risk sciences, and are coping more successfully with pandemic coronavirus. Others have failed to implement these old lessons, which our data suggest still apply. While these failures may reinforce cynicism about political and public enthusiasm for accepting science, comparisons between the coronavirus pandemic and climate change may also foster greater aspirations for collective action.en_US
dc.language.isoengen_US
dc.rightsAttribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 Internasjonal*
dc.rights.urihttp://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/deed.no*
dc.subjectCoronavirusen_US
dc.subjectclimate changeen_US
dc.subjectrisk comparisonsen_US
dc.subjectrisk communicationen_US
dc.subjectKoronavirusen_US
dc.subjectklimaendringeren_US
dc.subjectrisikoen_US
dc.titleComparative risk science for the coronavirus pandemicen_US
dc.typePeer revieweden_US
dc.typeJournal articleen_US
dc.description.versionacceptedVersionen_US
dc.subject.nsiVDP::Samfunnsvitenskap: 200en_US
dc.source.pagenumber902-911en_US
dc.source.volume23en_US
dc.source.journalJournal of Risk Researchen_US
dc.source.issue7-8en_US
dc.identifier.doi10.1080/13669877.2020.1756384
dc.identifier.cristin1877954
cristin.ispublishedtrue
cristin.fulltextpostprint
cristin.qualitycode1


Files in this item

Thumbnail

This item appears in the following Collection(s)

Show simple item record

Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 Internasjonal
Except where otherwise noted, this item's license is described as Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 Internasjonal